Tuesday, August 12, 2008

Attachment Disorder, Care Of CPS

I looked something up tonight which someone mentioned recently: attachment disorder.

I realized, this is exactly what has happened to my son, care of CPS. What they did to him, created a number of problems which didn't exist before. My son is now vulnerable and has been damaged, and already shows signs of this damage and is likely to show further signs in the future.

I practiced attachment parenting and what CPS did, in their removal of my son from his normal and loving home environment, created attachment disorder. The fact that I practiced such a bonding type of parenting to begin with, likely only made the traumatic separation even worse, and magnified the severity of the issues.

I am going to copy and paste a wiki clip on attachment disorder here.

Wikipedia is sustained by people like you. Please donate today.
Attachment disorder
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia


The term attachment disorder is most often used to describe emotional and behavioral problems of young children.
Attachment disorder is a broad term intended to describe disorders of mood, behavior, and social relationships arising from a failure to form normal attachments to primary care giving figures in early childhood, resulting in problematic social expectations and behaviors. Such a failure would result from unusual early experiences of neglect, abuse, abrupt separation from caregivers after about six months but before about three years, frequent change of caregivers or excessive numbers of caregivers, or lack of caregiver responsiveness to child communicative efforts. A problematic history of social relationships occurring after about age three may be distressing to a child, but does not result in attachment disorder.
The term attachment disorder is most often used to describe emotional and behavioral problems of young children, but is sometimes applied to school-age children or even to adults. The specific difficulties implied depend on the age of the individual being assessed and a child's attachment-related behaviors may be very different with one familiar adult than with another, suggesting that the disorder is within the relationship and interactions of the two people rather than an aspect of one or the other personality.[1] No list of symptoms can legitimately be presented but generally the term attachment disorder refers to the absence or distortion of age-appropriate social behaviors with adults. For example, in a toddler, attachment-disordered behavior could include a failure to stay near familiar adults in a strange environment or to be comforted by contact with a familiar person, whereas in a six-year-old attachment-disordered behavior might involve excessive friendliness and inappropriate approaches to strangers.
There are currently two main areas of theory and practice relating to the definition and diagnosis of attachment disorder, and considerable discussion about a broader definition altogether. The first main area is based on scientific enquiry, is found in academic journals and books and pays close attention to attachment theory. It is described in ICD-10 as reactive attachment disorder, or "RAD" for the inhibited form, and disinhibited attachment disorder, or "DAD" for the disinhibited form. In DSM-IV-TR both comparable inhibited and disinhibited types are called reactive attachment disorder or "RAD". The second area is controversial and is found in clinical practice, on websites and in books and publications, has little or no evidence base and makes controversial claims relating to a basis in attachment theory.[2] The use of these controversial diagnoses of attachment disorder is linked to the use of controversial attachment therapies to treat them.[3]
Thirdly, some authors have suggested that attachment, as an aspect of emotional development, is better assessed along a spectrum than considered to fall into two non-overlapping categories. This spectrum would have at one end the characteristics called secure attachment; midway along the range of disturbance would be insecure or other undesirable attachment styles; at the other extreme would be non-attachment.[4] Agreement has not yet been reached with respect to diagnostic criteria.[5]
Finally, the term is also sometimes used to cover difficulties arising in relation to various attachment styles which may not be disorders in the clinical sense.
The present article will consider ways of looking at attachment-related problems ranging from mild to serious.
Contents
[hide]
1 Attachment and attachment disorder
2 Classification
3 Boris and Zeanah's typology
4 Problems of attachment style
5 Diagnosis
6 Alternative diagnosis
7 Treatment
8 Possible mechanisms
9 See also
10 Notes
11 References
12 Further reading
[edit]Attachment and attachment disorder

Main article: Attachment theory
Main article: Attachment in children
Attachment theory is primarily an evolutionary and ethological theory. In relation to infants, it primarily consists of proximity seeking to an attachment figure in the face of threat, for the purpose of survival.[6] Although an attachment is a "tie" it is not synonymous with love and affection although they often go together and a healthy attachment is considered to be an important foundation of all subsequent relationships. Infants become attached to adults who are sensitive and responsive in social interactions with the infant, and who remain as consistent caregivers for some time. Parental responses lead to the development of patterns of attachment which in turn lead to 'internal working models' which will guide the individual's feelings, thoughts and expectations in later relationships.[7]
In the clinical sense, a disorder is a condition requiring treatment as opposed to risk factors for subsequent disorders.[8] There is a lack of consensus about the precise meaning of the term 'attachment disorder' although there is general agreement that such disorders only arise following early adverse caregiving experiences. Reactive attachment disorder indicates the absence of either or both the main aspects of proximity seeking to an identified attachment figure. This can occur either in institutions, or with repeated changes of caregiver, or from extremely neglectful primary caregivers who show persistent disregard for the child's basic attachment needs after the age of 6 months. Current official classifications of RAD under DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 are largely based on this understanding of the nature of attachment.
The words attachment style refer to the various types of attachment arising from early care experiences, called secure, anxious-ambivalent, anxious-avoidant, (all organized), and disorganized. Some of these styles are more problematical than others, and, although they are not disorders in the clinical sense, are sometimes discussed under the term 'attachment disorder'.
Discussion of 'disorganized attachment' style sometimes includes this style under the rubric of attachment disorders because disorganized attachment is seen as the beginning of a developmental trajectory that will take the individual ever further from the normal range, culminating in actual disorders of thought, behavior, or mood.[9] Early intervention for disorganized attachment, or other problematic styles, is directed toward changing the trajectory of development to provide a better outcome later in the person's life.
Zeanah and colleagues proposed an alternative set of criteria (see below) of three categories of attachment disorder, namely "no discriminated attachment figure", "secure base distortions" and "disrupted attachment disorder". These classifications consider that a disorder is a variation that requires treatment rather than an individual difference within the normal range.[10]
[edit]Classification

Main article: Reactive attachment disorder
ICD-10 describes Reactive Attachment Disorder of Childhood, known as RAD, and Disinhibited Disorder of Childhood, less well known as DAD. DSM-IV-TR also describes Reactive Attachment Disorder of Infancy or Early Childhood. They divide this into two subtypes, Inhibited Type and Disinhibited Type, both known as RAD. The two classifications are similar and both include:
markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relatedness in most contexts,
the disturbance is not accounted for solely by developmental delay and does not meet the criteria for Pervasive Developmental Disorder,
onset before 5 years of age,
requires a history of significant neglect, and
implicit lack of identifiable, preferred attachment figure.
ICD-10 includes in its diagnosis psychological and physical abuse and injury in addition to neglect. This is somewhat controversial, being a commission rather than omission and because abuse of itself does not lead to attachment disorder.
The inhibited form is described as "a failure to initiate or respond...to most social interactions, as manifest by excessively inhibited responses" and such infants do not seek and accept comfort at times of threat, alarm or distress, thus failing to maintain 'proximity', an essential element of attachment behavior. The disinhibited form shows "indiscriminate sociability...excessive familiarity with relative strangers" (DSM-IV-TR) and therefore a lack of 'specificity', the second basic element of attachment behavior. The ICD-10 descriptions are comparable. 'Disinhibited' and 'inhibited' are not opposites in terms of attachment disorder and can co-exist in the same child. The inhibited form has a greater tendency to ameliorate with an appropriate caregiver whilst the disinhibited form is more enduring.[11]
Whilst RAD is likely to occur following neglectful and abusive childcare, there should be no automatic diagnosis on this basis alone as children can form stable attachments and social relationships despite marked abuse and neglect. Abuse can occur alongside the required factors but on its own does not explain attachment disorder. Experiences of abuse are associated with the development of disorganised attachment, in which the child prefers a familiar caregiver, but responds to that person in an unpredictable and somewhat bizarre way. Within official classifications, attachment disorganization is a risk factor but not in itself an attachment disorder. Further although attachment disorders tend to occur in the context of some institutions, repeated changes of primary caregiver or extremely neglectful identifiable primary caregivers who show persistent disregard for the child's basic attachment needs, not all children raised in these conditions develop an attachment disorder.[12]
[edit]Boris and Zeanah's typology

Many leading attachment theorists, such as Zeanah and Leiberman, have recognized the limitations of the DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10 criteria and proposed broader diagnostic criteria. There is as yet no official consensus on these criteria. The APSAC Taskforce recognised in its recommendations that "attachment problems extending beyond RAD, are a real and appropriate concern for professionals working with children", and set out recommendations for assessment.[13]
Boris and Zeanah (1999),[14] have offered an approach to attachment disorders that considers cases where children have had no opportunity to form an attachment, those where there is a distorted relationship, and those where an existing attachment has been abruptly disrupted. This would significantly extend the definition beyond the ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR definitions because those definitions are limited to situations where the child has no attachment or no attachment to a specified attachment figure.
Boris and Zeanah use the term "disorder of attachment" to indicate a situation in which a young child has no preferred adult caregiver. Such children may be indiscriminately sociable and approach all adults, whether familiar or not; alternatively, they may be emotionally withdrawn and fail to seek comfort from anyone. This type of attachment problem is parallel to Reactive Attachment Disorder as defined in DSM and ICD in its inhibited and disinhibited forms as described above.
Boris and Zeanah also describe a condition they term "secure base distortion". In this situation, the child has a preferred familiar caregiver, but the relationship is such that the child cannot use the adult for safety while gradually exploring the environment. Such children may endanger themselves, may cling to the adult, may be excessively compliant, or may show role reversals in which they care for or punish the adult.
The third type of disorder discussed by Boris and Zeanah is termed "disrupted attachment". This type of problem, which is not covered under other approaches to disordered attachment, results from an abrupt separation or loss of a familiar caregiver to whom attachment has developed. The young child's reaction to such a loss is parallel to the grief reaction of an older person, with progressive changes from protest (crying and searching) to despair, sadness, and withdrawal from communication or play, and finally detachment from the original relationship and recovery of social and play activities.
[edit]Problems of attachment style

Main article: Attachment theory
Main article: Attachment in children
The majority of 12-month-old children can tolerate brief separations from familiar caregivers and are quickly comforted when the caregivers return. These children also use familiar people as a "secure base" and return to them periodically when exploring a new situation. Such children are said to have a secure attachment style, and characteristically continue to develop well both cognitively and emotionally.
Smaller numbers of children show less positive development at age 12 months. Their less desirable attachment styles may be predictors of poor later social development. Although these children's behavior at 12 months is not a serious problem, they appear to be on developmental trajectories that will end in poor social skills and relationships. Because attachment styles may serve as predictors of later development, it may be appropriate to think of certain attachment styles as part of the range of attachment disorders.
Insecure attachment styles in toddlers involve unusual reunions after separation from a familiar person. The children may snub the returning caregiver, or may go to the person but then resist being picked up. These children are more likely to have later social problems with peers and teachers, but some of them spontaneously develop better ways of interacting with other people.
A small group of toddlers show a distressing way of reuniting after a separation. Called a disorganized/disoriented style, this reunion pattern can involve looking dazed or frightened, freezing in place, backing toward the caregiver or approaching with head sharply averted, or showing other behaviors that seem to imply fearfulness of the person who is being sought.[15] Disorganized attachment has been considered a major risk factor for child psychopathology, as it appears to interfere with regulation or tolerance of negative emotions and may thus foster aggressive behavior.[16] Disorganized patterns of attachment have the strongest links to concurrent and subsequent psychopathology, and considerable research has demonstrated both within-the-child and environmental correlates of disorganized attachment.[17]
[edit]Diagnosis

Main article: Attachment measures
Recognised assessment methods of attachment styles, difficulties or disorders include the Strange Situation procedure (Mary Ainsworth),[18][19][20] the separation and reunion procedure and the Preschool Assessment of Attachment ("PAA"),[21] the Observational Record of the Caregiving Environment ("ORCE")[22] and the Attachment Q-sort ("AQ-sort").[23] More recent research also uses the Disturbances of Attachment Interview or "DAI" developed by Smyke and Zeanah, (1999).[24] This is a semi-structured interview designed to be administered by clinicians to caregivers. It covers 12 items, namely having a discriminated, preferred adult, seeking comfort when distressed, responding to comfort when offered, social and emotional reciprocity, emotional regulation, checking back after venturing away from the care giver, reticence with unfamiliar adults, willingness to go off with relative strangers, self endangering behavior, excessive clinging, vigilance/hypercompliance and role reversal.
[edit]Alternative diagnosis

Main article: Attachment therapy
In the absence of officially recognized diagnostic criteria, and beyond the ambit of the discourse on a broader set of criteria discussed above, the term attachment disorder has been increasingly used by some clinicians to refer to a broader set of children whose behavior may be affected by lack of a primary attachment figure, a seriously unhealthy attachment relationship with a primary caregiver, or a disrupted attachment relationship.[25] Although there are no studies examining diagnostic accuracy, concern is expressed as to the potential for over-diagnosis based on broad checklists and 'snapshots'.[26]
A common feature of this form of diagnosis within attachment therapy is the use of extensive lists of "symptoms" which include many behaviours that are likely to be a consequence of neglect or abuse, but are not related to attachment, or not related to any clinical disorder at all. Such lists have been described as "wildly inclusive".[27] The APSAC Taskforce (2006) gives examples of such lists ranging across multiple domains from some elements within the DSM-IV criteria to entirely non-specific behavior such as developmental lags, destructive behaviors, refusal to make eye contact,cruelty to animals and siblings, lack of cause and effect thinking, preoccupation with fire, blood, and gore, poor peer relationships, stealing, lying, lack of a conscience, persistent nonsense questions or incessant chatter, poor impulse control, abnormal speech patterns, fighting for control over everything, and hoarding or gorging on food. Some checklists suggest that among infants, “prefers dad to mom” or “wants to hold the bottle as soon as possible” are indicative of attachment problems. The APSAC Taskforce expresses concern that high rates of false positive diagnoses are virtually certain and that posting these types of lists on Web sites that also serve as marketing tools may lead many parents or others to conclude inaccurately that their children have attachment disorders."[28]
[edit]Treatment

Main article: Child therapy
Main article: Attachment therapy
There is a variety of mainstream prevention programs and treatment approaches for attachment disorder, attachment problems and moods or behaviors considered to be potential problems within the context of attachment theory. All such approaches for infants and younger children concentrate on increasing the responsiveness and sensitivity of the caregiver, or if that is not possible, changing the caregiver.[29][30][31] Such approaches include 'Watch, wait and wonder,'[32] manipulation of sensitive responsiveness,[33][34] modified 'Interaction Guidance,'.[35] 'Preschool Parent Psychotherapy,'.[36] 'Circle of Security',[37][38] Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up (ABC),[39] the New Orleans Intervention,[40][41][42] and Parent-Child psychotherapy.[43] Other known treatment methods include Developmental, Individual-difference, Relationship-based therapy (DIR) (also referred to as Floor Time) by Stanley Greenspan, although DIR is primarily directed to treatment of pervasive developmental disorders[44] Some of these approaches, such as that suggested by Dozier, consider the attachment status of the adult caregiver to play an important role in the development of the emotional connection between adult and child. This includes foster parents as children with poor attachment experiences often do not elicit appropriate caregiver responses from their attachment behaviors despite 'normative' care.[45]
There is also a considerable variety of treatments for alleged attachment disorders diagnosed on the controversial alternative basis outlined above, popularly known as attachment therapy. These therapies have little or no evidence base and vary from talking or play therapies to more extreme forms of physical and coercive techniques, of which the best known are holding therapy, rebirthing, rage-reduction and the Evergreen model. In general these therapies are aimed at adopted or fostered children with a view to creating attachment in these children to their new caregivers. Critics maintain these therapies are not based on an accepted version of attachment theory.[46] The theoretical base is broadly a combination of regression and catharsis, accompanied by parenting methods which emphasise obedience and parental control.[47] These therapies concentrate on changing the child rather than the caregiver.[48] An estimated six children have died as a consequence of the more coercive forms of such treatments and the application of the accompanying parenting techniques.[49][50][51]
[edit]Possible mechanisms

One study has reported a connection between a specific genetic marker and disorganized attachment (not RAD) associated with problems of parenting.[52] Another author has compared atypical social behavior in genetic conditions such as Williams syndrome with behaviors symptomatic of RAD.[53]
Typical attachment development begins with unlearned infant reactions to social signals from caregivers. The ability to send and receive social communications through facial expressions, gestures and voice develops with social experience by seven to nine months. This makes it possible for an infant to interpret messages of calm or alarm from face or voice. At about eight months, infants typically begin to respond with fear to unfamiliar or startling situations, and to look to the faces of familiar caregivers for information that either justifies or soothes their fear. This developmental combination of social skills and the emergence of fear reactions results in attachment behavior such as proximity-seeking, if a familiar, sensitive, responsive, and cooperative adult is available. Further developments in attachment, such as negotiation of separation in the toddler and preschool period, depend on factors such as the caregiver's interaction style and ability to understand the child's emotional communications.[54]
With insensitive or unresponsive caregivers, or frequent changes, an infant may have few experiences that encourage proximity seeking to a familiar person. An infant who experiences fear but who cannot find comforting information in an adult's face and voice may develop atypical ways of coping with fearfulness such as the maintenance of distance from adults, or the seeking of proximity to all adults. These symptoms accord with the DSM criteria for reactive attachment disorder.[55] Either of these behavior patterns may create a developmental trajectory leading ever farther from typical attachment processes such as the development of an internal working model of social relationships that facilitates both the giving and the receiving of care from others.[56][57]
Atypical development of fearfulness, with a constitutional tendency either to excessive or inadequate fear reactions, might be necessary before an infant is vulnerable to the effects of poor attachment experiences.[58]
Alternatively, the two variations of RAD may develop from the same inability to develop "stranger-wariness" due to inadequate care. Appropriate fear responses may only be able to develop after an infant has first begun to form a selective attachment. An infant who is not in a position do this cannot afford not to show interest in any person as they may be potential attachment figures. Faced with a swift succession of carers the child may have no opportunity to form a selective attachment until the possible biological-determined sensitive period for developing stranger-wariness has passed. It is thought this process may lead to the disinhibited form.[59]
In the inhibited form infants behave as if their attachment system has been "switched off". However the innate capacity for attachment behavior cannot be lost. This may explain why children diagnosed with the inhibited form of RAD from institutions almost invariably go on to show formation of attachment behavior to good carers. However children who suffer the inhibited form as a consequence of neglect and frequent changes of caregiver continue to show the inhibited form for far longer when placed in families.[60]
Additionally, the development of Theory of Mind may play a role in emotional development. Theory of Mind is the ability to know that the experience of knowledge and intention lies behind human actions such as facial expressions. Although it is reported that very young infants respond differently to humans and objects, Theory of Mind develops relatively gradually and possibly results from predictable interactions with adults. However, some ability of this kind must be in place before mutual communication through gaze or other gesture can occur, as it does by seven to nine months. Some early emotional disorders, such as autism, have been attributed to the absence of the mental functions that underlie Theory of Mind. It is possible that the congenital absence of this ability, or the lack of experiences with caregivers who communicate in a predictable fashion, could underlie the development of reactive attachment disorder.[61][62]
[edit]See also

Reactive attachment disorder
Attachment theory
Attachment therapy
Candace Newmaker
Child therapy
Emotional dysregulation
John Bowlby
Michael and Sharen Gravelle
Stanley Greenspan
http://www.isabellight.blogspot.com/
[edit]Notes

^ Zeanah, 2005
^ Prior & Glaser p 183
^ Chaffin et al (2006) p78
^ O'Connor & Zeanah, (2003)
^ Chaffin et al p. (2006)
^ Bowlby (1970) p 181
^ Bretherton & Munholland (1999) p 89
^ AACAP 2005, p1208
^ Levy K.N. et al (2005)
^ Prior & Glaser (2006) p 223
^ Prior & Glaser 2006, p. 220-221.
^ Prior & Glaser (2006) p218-219
^ Chaffin (2006) p 86
^ Boris & Zeannah (1999)
^ Mercer, J (2006) p 107
^ VanIJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg (2003)
^ Zeanah et al (2003)
^ Ainsworth (1978),
^ Main & Solomon (1986), pp.95-124.
^ Main & Solomon (1990), pp. 121-160.
^ Crittenden (1992)
^ National Institute of Child Health and Human Development(1996)
^ Waters and Deane (1985)
^ Smyke and Zeanah (1999)
^ Chaffin et al, (2006) p 81
^ Chaffin et al (2006) p 82
^ Prior & Glaser (2006) p186-187
^ Chaffin (2006) p 82
^ Prior & Glaser (2006), p. 231.
^ AACAP (2005) p. 17-18.
^ BakermansKranenburg et al. (2003) A meta-analysis of early interventions.
^ Cohen et al. (1999)
^ van den Boom (1994)
^ van den Boom (1995)
^ Benoit et al. (2001)
^ Toth et al. (2002)
^ Marvin et al (2002)
^ Cooper et al (2005)
^ Dozier et al (2005)
^ Larrieu & Zeanah (1998)
^ Larrieu & Zeannah (2004)
^ Zeannah & Smyke (2005)
^ Leiberman et al. (2000), p. 432.
^ Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental & Learning Disorders. (2007). Dir/floortime model.
^ Dozier et al (2005)
^ Prior & Glaser (2006) p 262
^ Chaffin et al. 2006, p. 79–80. The APSAC Taskforce Report.
^ Chaffin et al (2006) p 79
^ Boris 2003
^ Mercer, Sarner & Rosa 2003
^ Zeanah 2003
^ Van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2006). "DRD4 7-repeat polymorphism moderates the association between maternal unresolved loss or trauma and infant disorganization". Attach Hum Dev 8 (4): 291–307. doi:10.1080/14616730601048159. PMID 17178609.
^ Zeanah CH (2007). "Reactive Attachment Disorder". In Narrow WE, First MB et al (Eds.) Gender and age consideration in psychiatric diagnosis. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. ISBN 0890422958.
^ Dozier M, Stovall KC, Albus KE, Bates B (2001). "Attachment for infants in foster care: the role of caregiver state of mind". Child Dev 72 (5): 1467–77. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00360. PMID 11699682.
^ DSM-IV American Psychiatric Association 1994
^ Mercer J, Sarner L and Rosa L (2003) Attachment Therapy on Trial: The Torture and Death of Candace Newmaker. Westport, CT: Praeger ISBN 0275976750, pp. 98–103.
^ Mercer (2006), pp. 64–70.
^ Marshall,, P.J.; Fox, N.A. (2005). "Relationship between behavioral reactivity at 4 months and attachment classification at 14 months in a selected sample". Infant Behavior and Development 28: 492–502. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.06.002. (inactive 2008-06-21). Retrieved on 2008-01-21.
^ Prior and Glaser p.
^ Prior and Glaser p.
^ Mercer (2006) p.
^ Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist EL, Target M (2006). Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of Self. Other Press (NY) ISBN 1892746344
[edit]References

Ainsworth. Mary D., Blehar, M., Waters, E., &b Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-89859-461-8.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP)(2005). Practice Parameter for the Assessment and Treatment of Children and Adolescents with Reactive Attachment Disorder of Infancy and Early Childhood.(PDF). Boris, N. & Zeanah, C. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Nov; 44:1206–1219 (Guideline at [1])
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., van IJzendoorn, M. and Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. (PDF) Psychological Bulletin 129, 195–215
Benoit, D., Madigan, S., Lecce, S., Shea, B. and Goldberg, S. (2001). Atypical maternal behaviour toward feeding disordered infants before and after intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal 22, 611–626. DOI: 10.1002/imhj.1022 [2]
Boris, N.W. and Zeanah, C.H. (1999). Disturbance and disorders of attachment in infancy: An overview. Infant Mental Health Journal. 20, 1–9. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199921)20:1<1::AID-IMHJ1>3.0.CO;2-V[3]
Boris NW (2003). "Attachment, aggression and holding: a cautionary tale". Attach Hum Dev 5 (3): 245–7. doi:10.1080/14616730310001593947. PMID 12944217.
Bowlby J [1969] 2nd edition (1999). Attachment, Attachment and Loss (vol. 1), New York: Basic Books. LCCN 00266879; NLM 8412414. ISBN 0-465-00543-8 (pbk). OCLC 11442968.
Bretherton, I. and Munholland, K., A. (1999). Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships: A Construct Revisited. In Cassidy, J. and Shaver, P., R. (eds.) Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications..pp.89-111. Guilford Press ISBN 1-57230-087-6.
Chaffin M, Hanson R, Saunders BE, et al (2006). "Report of the APSAC task force on attachment therapy, reactive attachment disorder, and attachment problems". Child Maltreat 11 (1): 76–89. doi:10.1177/1077559505283699. PMID 16382093.
Cohen,N., Muir, E., Lojkasek, M., Muir, R., Parker, C., Barwick, M. and Brown, M. (1999) 'Watch,wait and wonder: testing the effectiveness of a new approach to mother-infant psychotherapy.' Infant Mental health Journal 20, 429-451.
Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., Powell, B. and Marvin, R. (2007). The Circle of Security Intervention; differential diagnosis and differential treatment. In Berlin, L.J., Ziv, Y., Amaya-Jackson, L. and Greenberg, M.T. (eds.) Enhancing Early Attachments; Theory, research, intervention, and policy. The Guilford Press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. pp 127-151. ISBN 1593854706.
Crittenden, P. M. (1992). Quality of attachment in the preschool years. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 209-241.[4]
Dozier,M., Lindheim,O. and Ackerman, J., P. 'Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-Up: An intervention targeting empirically identified needs of foster infants'. In Berlin, L.J., Ziv, Y., Amaya-Jackson, L. and Greenberg, M.T. (eds) Enhancing Early Attachments; Theory, research, intervention, and policy The Guilford press. Duke series in Child Development and Public Policy. pp 178 - 194. (2005)ISBN 1593854706 (pbk)
Interdisciplinary Council on Developmental & Learning Disorders. (2007). Dir/floortime model.
Zeanah CH, Larrieu JA (1998). "Intensive intervention for maltreated infants and toddlers in foster care". Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 7 (2): 357–71. PMID 9894069.
Larrieu,J.A., & Zeanah,C.H. (2004). Treating infant-parent relationships in the context of maltreatment: An integrated, systems approach. In A.Saner, S. McDonagh, & K. Roesenblaum (eds.) Treating parent-infant relationship problems (pp. 243-264). New York: Guilford Press ISBN 1593852452
Levy KN, Meehan KB, Weber M, Reynoso J, Clarkin JF (2005). "Attachment and borderline personality disorder: implications for psychotherapy". Psychopathology 38 (2): 64–74. doi:10.1159/000084813. PMID 15802944.
Lieberman, A.F., Silverman, R., Pawl, J.H. (2000). Infant-parent psychotherapy. In C.H. Zeanah, Jr. (ed.) Handbook of infant mental health (2nd ed.) (p. 432). New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 1593851715
Main, M. and Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern: procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior. In T. Braxelton and M.Yogman (eds) Affective development in infancy, (pp. 95-124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex ISBN 0893913456
Main, M. and Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti and E. Cummings (eds) Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention, (pp. 121-160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0226306305.
Mercer, J., Sarner, L., & Rosa, L. (2003). Attachment therapy on trial: The torture and death of Candace Newmaker. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group, Inc. ISBN 0275976750
Mercer, J (2006) Understanding Attachment: Parenting, child care and emotional development. Westport, CT: Praeger ISBN 0275982173
Marvin, R., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K. and Powell, B. The Circle of Security project: Attachment-based intervention with caregiver – pre-school child dyads. Attachment & Human Development Vol 4 No 1 April 2002 107–124.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (1996). Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 269-306
O'Connor TG, Zeanah CH (2003). "Attachment disorders: assessment strategies and treatment approaches". Attach Hum Dev 5 (3): 223–44. doi:10.1080/14616730310001593974. PMID 12944216.
Prior, V., Glaser, D. Understanding Attachment and Attachment Disorders: Theory, Evidence and Practice (2006). Child and Adolescent Mental Health Series. Jessica Kingsley Publishers London ISBN 1843102455 OCLC 70663735
Smyke, A. and Zeanah, C. (1999). Disturbances of Attachment Interview. Available on the Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry website at [5]
Toth, S., Maughan, A., Manly, J., Spagnola, M. and Cicchetti, D. (2002) The relative efficacy of two in altering maltreated preschool children's representational models: implications for attachment theory. Development and psychopathology 14, 877–908. doi:10.1017.S095457940200411X
van den Boom, D. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: an experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development 65, 1457–1477.doi:10.2307/1131277
van den Boom DC (1995). "Do first-year intervention effects endure? Follow-up during toddlerhood of a sample of Dutch irritable infants". Child Dev 66 (6): 1798–816. PMID 8556900.
Van Ijzendoorn M, Bakermans-Kranenburg M. Attachment disorders and disorganized attachment: Similar and different Attachment & Human Development, Volume 5, Number 3, September 2003 , pp. 313–320(8) DOI: 10.1080/14616730310001593938 [6]
Waters, E. and Deane, K (1985). Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. In I. Bretherton and E. Waters (Eds) Growing pains of attachment theory and research: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 50, Serial No. 209 (1–2), 41–65 [7]
O'Connor TG, Zeanah CH (2003). "Attachment disorders: assessment strategies and treatment approaches". Attach Hum Dev 5 (3): 223–44. doi:10.1080/14616730310001593974. PMID 12944216.
Zeanah CH, Keyes A, Settles L (2003). "Attachment relationship experiences and childhood psychopathology". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1008: 22–30. doi:10.1196/annals.1301.003. PMID 14998869.
Zeanah, C., H. and Smyke, A., T. "Building Attachment Relationships Following Maltreatment and Severe Deprivation" In Berlin,L.,J., Ziv, Y., Amaya-Jackson, L. and Greenberg, M., T. Enhancing Early Attachments; Theory, research, intervention, and policy The Guilford Press, 2005 pps 195-216 ISBN 1593854706 (pbk)
[edit]Further reading

Holmes, J (2001). The Search for the Secure Base. Philadelphia: Brunner-Routledge. ISBN 1583911529
Cassidy, J; Shaver, P (eds.) (1999). Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 1-57230-087-6.
Zeanah, CH (ed.) (1993). Handbook of Infant Mental Health. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 1593851715
Bowlby, J (1988). A Secure Base: Parent-Child Attachment and Healthy Human Development. London: Routledge; New York: Basic Books. ISBN 0-415-00640-6.
[show]
v • d • e
WHO ICD-10 mental and behavioral disorders (F · 290–319)






[show]
v • d • e
Attachment theory
Categories: Attachment theory | Human development

No comments: